There’s one key difference between kids who excel at math and those who don’t
“I’m just not a math person.”
We
hear it all the time. And we’ve had enough. Because we believe that the
idea of “math people” is the most self-destructive idea in America
today. The truth is, you probably are a math person, and by
thinking otherwise, you are possibly hamstringing your own career.
Worse, you may be helping to perpetuate a pernicious myth that is
harming underprivileged children—the myth of inborn genetic math
ability.
Is math ability genetic? Sure, to some degree.
Terence Tao, UCLA’s famous virtuoso mathematician, publishes dozens of
papers in top journals every year, and is sought out by researchers
around the world to help with the hardest parts of their theories.
Essentially none of us could ever be as good at math as Terence Tao, no
matter how hard we tried or how well we were taught. But here’s the
thing: We don’t have to! For high school math, inborn talent is just much less important than hard work, preparation, and self-confidence.
How
do we know this? First of all, both of us have taught math for many
years—as professors, teaching assistants, and private tutors. Again and
again, we have seen the following pattern repeat itself:
Different
kids with different levels of preparation come into a math class. Some
of these kids have parents who have drilled them on math from a young
age, while others never had that kind of parental input.
- On the first few tests, the well-prepared kids get perfect scores, while the unprepared kids get only what they could figure out by winging it—maybe 80 or 85%, a solid B.
- The unprepared kids, not realizing that the top scorers were well-prepared, assume that genetic ability was what determined the performance differences. Deciding that they “just aren’t math people,” they don’t try hard in future classes, and fall further behind.
- The well-prepared kids, not realizing that the B students were simply unprepared, assume that they are “math people,” and work hard in the future, cementing their advantage.
Thus, people’s belief that math ability can’t change becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy.
The
idea that math ability is mostly genetic is one dark facet of a larger
fallacy that intelligence is mostly genetic. Academic psychology
journals are well stocked with papers studying the world view that lies
behind the kind of self-fulfilling prophecy we just described. For
example, Purdue University psychologist Patricia Linehan writes:
A body of research on conceptions of ability has shown two orientations toward ability. Students with an Incremental orientation believe ability (intelligence) to be malleable, a quality that increases with effort. Students with an Entity orientation believe ability to be nonmalleable, a fixed quality of self that does not increase with effort.
The “entity orientation” that says “You are smart or not, end of story,” leads to bad outcomes—a result that has been confirmed by many other studies. (The relevance for math is shown by researchers at Oklahoma City who recently found that belief in inborn math ability may be responsible for much of the gender gap in mathematics.)
Psychologists Lisa Blackwell, Kali Trzesniewski, and Carol Dweck presented these alternatives to determine people’s beliefs about intelligence:
- You have a certain amount of intelligence, and you really can’t do much to change it.
- You can always greatly change how intelligent you are.
They
found that students who agreed that “You can always greatly change how
intelligent you are” got higher grades. But as Richard Nisbett recounts
in his book Intelligence and How to Get It, they did something even more remarkable:
Dweck and her colleagues then tried to convince a group of poor minority junior high school students that intelligence is highly malleable and can be developed by hard work…that learning changes the brain by forming new…connections and that students are in charge of this change process.
The
results? Convincing students that they could make themselves smarter by
hard work led them to work harder and get higher grades. The
intervention had the biggest effect for students who started out
believing intelligence was genetic. (A control group, who were taught
how memory works, showed no such gains.)
But
improving grades was not the most dramatic effect, “Dweck reported that
some of her tough junior high school boys were reduced to tears by the
news that their intelligence was substantially under their control.” It
is no picnic going through life believing you were born dumb—and are
doomed to stay that way.
For almost everyone, believing that you were born dumb—and are doomed to stay that way—is believing a lie. IQ itself can improve with hard work.
Because the truth may be hard to believe, here is a set of links about
some excellent books to convince you that most people can become smart
in many ways, if they work hard enough:
- The Art of Learning by Josh Weitzkin
- Moonwalking with Einstein by Joshua Foer
- The Talent Code by Daniel Coyle
- Talent is Overrated by Geoff Colvin
So
why do we focus on math? For one thing, math skills are increasingly
important for getting good jobs these days—so believing you can’t learn
math is especially self-destructive. But we also believe that math is
the area where America’s “fallacy of inborn ability” is the most
entrenched. Math is the great mental bogeyman of an unconfident America.
If we can convince you that anyone can learn math, it should be a short
step to convincing you that you can learn just about anything, if you
work hard enough.
Is
America more susceptible than other nations to the dangerous idea of
genetic math ability? Here our evidence is only anecdotal, but we
suspect that this is the case. While American fourth and eighth graders score quite well in international math comparisons—beating countries like Germany, the UK and Sweden—our high-schoolers underperform those countries
by a wide margin. This suggests that Americans’ native ability is just
as good as anyone’s, but that we fail to capitalize on that ability
through hard work. In response to the lackluster high school math
performance, some influential voices in American education policy have
suggested simply teaching less math—for example, Andrew Hacker has called
for algebra to no longer be a requirement. The subtext, of course, is
that large numbers of American kids are simply not born with the ability
to solve for x.
We
believe that this approach is disastrous and wrong. First of all, it
leaves many Americans ill-prepared to compete in a global marketplace
with hard-working foreigners. But even more importantly, it may
contribute to inequality. A great deal of research has shown that
technical skills in areas like software are increasingly making the
difference between America’s upper middle class and its working class.
While we don’t think education is a cure-all for inequality, we
definitely believe that in an increasingly automated workplace,
Americans who give up on math are selling themselves short.
Too
many Americans go through life terrified of equations and mathematical
symbols. We think what many of them are afraid of is “proving”
themselves to be genetically inferior by failing to instantly comprehend
the equations (when, of course, in reality, even a math professor would
have to read closely). So they recoil from anything that looks like
math, protesting: “I’m not a math person.” And so they exclude
themselves from quite a few lucrative career opportunities.
We believe that this has to stop. Our view is shared by economist and
writer Allison Schrager, who has written two wonderful columns in Quartz
(here and here), that echo many of our views.
One way to help Americans excel at math is to copy the approach of the Japanese, Chinese, and Koreans. In Intelligence and How to Get It, Nisbett describes how the educational systems of East Asian countries focus more on hard work than on inborn talent:
1. “Children in Japan go to school about 240 days a year, whereas children in the United States go to school about 180 days a year.”
2. “Japanese high school students of the 1980s studied 3 ½ hours a day, and that number is likely to be, if anything, higher today.”
3. “[The inhabitants of Japan and Korea] do not need to read this book to find out that intelligence and intellectual accomplishment are highly malleable. Confucius set that matter straight twenty-five hundred years ago.”
4. “When they do badly at something, [Japanese, Koreans, etc.] respond by working harder at it.”
5. “Persistence in the face of failure is very much part of the Asian tradition of self-improvement. And [people in those countries] are accustomed to criticism in the service of self-improvement in situations where Westerners avoid it or resent it.”
We certainly don’t want America’s education system to copy everything
Japan does (and we remain agnostic regarding the wisdom of Confucius).
But it seems to us that an emphasis on hard work is a hallmark not just
of modern East Asia, but of America’s past as well. In returning to an
emphasis on effort, America would be returning to its roots, not just
copying from successful foreigners.
Besides cribbing a few tricks from the Japanese, we also have at least one American-style idea for making kids smarter: treat people who work hard at learning as heroes and role models.
We already venerate sports heroes who make up for lack of talent
through persistence and grit; why should our educational culture be any
different?
Math
education, we believe, is just the most glaring area of a slow and
worrying shift. We see our country moving away from a culture of hard
work toward a culture of belief in genetic determinism. In the debate
between “nature vs. nurture,” a critical third element—personal
perseverance and effort—seems to have been sidelined. We want to bring
it back, and we think that math is the best place to start.
Follow Miles on Twitter at @mileskimball. Follow Noah at @noahpinion. We welcome your comments at ideas@qz.com.
No comments:
Post a Comment